Za darmo

Decisiones económicas en los cambios hormonales de la mujer

Tekst
0
Recenzje
Oznacz jako przeczytane
Czcionka:Mniejsze АаWiększe Aa

Abdellaoui, M., Driouchi, A., & l’Haridon, O. (2011). Risk aversion elicitation: reconciling tractability and bias minimization. Theory and Decision, 71(1), 63-80.

Ariely, D., & Ramos, J. R. (2008). Las trampas del deseo: como controlar los impulsos irracionales que nos llevan al error. Barcelona: Ariel.

Artinger, F., Fleischhut, N., Levati, M. V., & Stevens, J. R. (2012). Cooperation in a risky environment: Decisions from experience in a stochastic social dilema. Proceedings of the 34th Conference of the Cognitive Science Society, 84–89.

Banco Mundial. (2015). Las nuevas compradoras reciclan, ahorran y cuidan el medio ambiente. Recuperado de: https://goo.gl/heVC4z

Baron, J. (2008). Thinking and Deciding. New York: Cambridge University Press.

Barreda-Tarrazona, I., Jaramillo-Gutiérrez, A., Navarro-Martínez, D., & Sabater-Grande, G. (2011). Risk actitude elicitation using a multi-lottery choice task: real vs. hipothetical incentives. Revista española de financiación y contabilidad, 40(152), 613-628.

Bechara, A. (2004). The role of emotion in decision-making: Evidence from neurological patients with orbitofrontal damage. Brain and Cognition, 55(1), 30-40.

Bechara, A. (2005). Decision making, impulse control and loss of willpower to resist drugs: a neurocognitive perspective. Nature neuroscience, 8(11), 1458-1463.

Bechara, A., Damasio, H., & Damasio, A. R. (2000). Emotion, decision making and the orbitofrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 10(3), 295–307.

Berg, J., Dickhaut, J., & McCabe, K. (1995). Trust, reciprocity, and social history. Games and economic behavior, 10(1), 122-142.

Bernoulli, D. (1954). Exposition of a New Theory on the Measurement of Risk. Econometrica, 22(1), 23-36.

Brennan, B. (2011). Why she buys: The new strategy for reaching the world’s most powerful consumers. New York: Crown Business.

Burton, S., Netemeyer, R. G., & Lichtenstein, D. R. (1995). Gender differences for appearance-related attitudes and behaviors: Implications for consumer welfare. Journal of Public Policy & Marketing, 14(1), 60-75.

Buser, T. (2011). Hormones and social preferences. Tinbergen Institute Discussion Paper, 11-046/3.

Buser, T. (2012). The impact of the menstrual cycle and hormonal contraceptives on competitiveness. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 83(1), 1-10.

Byrnes, J. P., Miller, D. C., & Schafer, W. D. (1999). Gender differences in risk taking: A meta-analysis. Psychological bulletin, 125(3), 367-383.

Carmona-Cañabate, S., & Moreno-Alcázar, A. (2014). Control ejecutivo, toma de decisiones, razonamiento y resolución de problemas. En: Redolar-Ripoll, D. (Ed.), Neurociencia cognitiva. (pp. 719-745). Madrid: Editorial Médica Panamericana.

Chavanne, T. J., & Gallup, G. G. (1998). Variation in risk taking behavior among female college students as a function of the menstrual cycle. Evolution and Human Behavior, 19(1), 27-32.

Chicaíza, L., García, M., & Romano, G. (2011). La aversión al riesgo en la toma de decisiones médicas: una revisión. Lecturas de economía, 75, 163-185.

Colombia. Ministerio de Hacienda y Crédito Público, Ministerio de Educación Nacional, Banco de la República, Superintendencia Financiera de Colombia, Fondo de Garantías de Instituciones Financieras, Fondo de Garantías de Entidades Cooperativas y Autorregulador del Mercado de Valores. (2010). Estrategia nacional de educación económica y financiera. Tomado de: https://goo.gl/7J7gRL

Corripio, F. (1984). Diccionario etimológico general de la lengua castellana (3a. ed.). Barcelona: Bruguera.

Croson, R., & Gneezy, U. (2009). Gender differences in preferences. Journal of Economic literature, 47(2), 448-474.

Curtis, H., Barnes, N., Schnek, A., & Flores, G. (2000). Biología. (6a. Ed.). España: Panamericana.

Damasio, A. R. (2001). El error de Descartes: la emoción, la razón y el cerebro humano. España: Crítica.

Delgado, C. D. (2014). Experimento controlado para explorar la aversión al riesgo en decisiones financieras. (Trabajo de grado). Universidad Industrial de Santander, Facultad de Ingenierías físico mecánicas, Bucaramanga.

Derntl, B., Pintzinger, N., Kryspin-Exner, I., & Schöpf, V. (2014). The impact of sex hormone concentrations on decision-making in females and males. Frontiers in neuroscience, 8, 352.

Doron, R., & Parot, F. (2007). Diccionario Akal de psicología. Madrid: Akal Editor.

Durante, K. M., & Arsena, A. R. (2015). Playing the field: The effect of fertility on women’s desire for variety. Journal of Consumer Research, 41(6), 1372-1391.

Durante, K. M., & Griskevicius, V. (2016). Evolution and consumer behavior. Current Opinion in Psychology, 10, 27-32.

Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Cantu, S. M., & Simpson, J. A. (2014). Money, status, and the ovulatory cycle. Journal of Marketing Research, 51(1), 27-39.

Durante, K. M., Griskevicius, V., Hill, S. E., Perilloux, C., & Li, N. P. (2011). Ovulation, female competition, and product choice: Hormonal influences on consumer behavior. Journal of Consumer Research, 37(6), 921-934.

Durante, K. M., Li, N. P., & Haselton, M. G. (2008). Changes in women’s choice of dress across the ovulatory cycle: Naturalistic and laboratory task-based evidence. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 34(11), 1451-1460.

Durante, K. M., & Saad, G. (2010). Ovulatory shifts in women’s social motives and behaviors: implications for corporate organizations. In: Stanton, A. A., Day, M., & Welpe, I. M. (Eds.). Neuroeconomics and the Firm (pp. 116-130). Massachusetts, USA: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Eichner, S. F., & Timpe, E. M. (2004). Urinary-Based Ovulation and Pregnancy: Point-of-Case Testing. Annals of Pharmacotherapy, 38, 325–31.

Eisenbruch, A. B., & Roney, J. R. (2016). Conception Risk and the Ultimatum Game: When Fertility is High, Women Demand More. Personality and Individual Differences, 98, 272-274.

Emol (08 de marzo de 2016). Consumo femenino: ¿Qué compran las mujeres latinoamericanas en internet? EMOL. Recuperado de https://goo.gl/RShwku

Escobar, M. E., Pipman, V., Arcari, A., Boulgoudjian, E., Kaselman, A., Pasqualini, T…, & Blanco, M. (2010). Trastornos del ciclo menstrual en la adolescencia. Archivos Argentinos de Pediatría, 108(4), 363-369.

Etchart-Vincent, N., & l’Haridon, O. (2011). Monetary incentives in the loss domain and behavior toward risk: An experimental comparison of three reward schemes including real losses. Journal of Risk and Uncertainty, 42(1), 61-83.

Fernández Arbeláez, J. I. (2013). Revisión sistemática sobre toma de decisiones como proceso de pensamiento 2002-2012. (Tesis de Maestría). Universidad de Antioquia, Facultad de Ciencias Sociales y Humanas, Medellín.

Fox, S. I., & Rivera, B. (2014). Fisiología humana. (13ª. ed.). México: McGraw-Hill.

Gangestad, S. W., Haselton, M. G., Welling, L. L., Gildersleeve, K., Pillsworth, E. G., Burriss, R. P., ... & Puts, D. A. (2016). How valid are assessments of conception probability in ovulatory cycle research? Evaluations, recommendations, and theoretical implications. Evolution and Human Behavior, 37(2), 85-96.

Gangestad, S. W., Simpson, J. A., Cousins, A. J., Garver-Apgar, C. E., & Niels Christensen, P. (2004). Women’s Preferences for Male Behavioral Displays Change Across the Menstrual Cycle. Psychological Science, 15(3), 203-207.

Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (1998). Menstrual cycle variation in women’s preferences for the scent of symmetrical men. Proceedings of the Royal Society B: Biological Sciences, 265(1399), 927-933.

Gangestad, S. W., & Thornhill, R. (2008). Human oestrus. Proceedings of the Royal Society of London B: Biological Sciences, 275(1638), 991-1000.

García Gallego, A., & Georgantzís, N. (2012). Sobre la toma de decisiones económicas bajo incertidumbre. Ciencia Cognitiva, 6(1), 27-29.

Gildersleeve, K., Haselton, M. G., & Fales, M. R. (2014). Do women’s mate preferences change across the ovulatory cycle? A meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin, 140(5), 1205-1259.

Goldstein, J. M., Jerram, M., Poldrack, R., Ahern, T., Kennedy, D. N., Seidman, L. J., & Makris, N. (2005). Hormonal cycle modulates arousal circuitry in women using functional magnetic resonance imaging. Journal of Neuroscience, 25(40), 9309-9316.

Grüner, S. (2017). Correlates of Multiple Switching in the Holt and Laury Procedure. Economics Bulletin, 37(1), 297-304.

Güth, W., & Kocher, M. G. (2014). More than thirty years of ultimatum bargaining experiments: Motives, variations, and a survey of the recent literature. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 108, 396–409.

Güth, W., Schmittberger, R., & Schwarze, B. (1982). An experimental analysis of ultimatum bargaining. Journal of economic behavior & organization, 3(4), 367-388.

 

Haegler, K., Zernecke, R., Kleemann, A. M., Albrecht, J., Pollatos, O., Brückmann, H., & Wiesmann, M. (2010). No fear no risk! Human risk behavior is affected by chemosensory anxiety signals. Neuropsychologia, 48(13), 3901-3908.

Hansson, S. O. (2007). Risk. Stanford Encyclopedia of Philosophy. Disponible en http://plato.stanford.edu/entries/risk/

Harrison, G. W. (2014). Real choices and hypothetical choices. In Hess, S., & Daly, A. (Eds.). Handbook of Choice Modelling. (pp. 236–254). Cheltenham: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Haselton, M. G., & Miller, G. F. (2006). Women’s Fertility across the Cycle Increases the Short Term Attractiveness of Creative Intelligence. Human Nature, 17(1), 50–73.

Hevia-Orozco, J., Sanz-Martín A., Hidalgo-Aguirre, R. M., Hernández-González, M., & Guevara, M. A. (2014). Evaluación de la toma de decisiones sociales a través del Ultimatum Game versión computarizada. Eneurobiología, 5(10), 1-11.

Hirschauer, N., Musshoff, O., Maart-Noelck, S. C., & Gruener, S. (2014). Eliciting risk attitudes–how to avoid mean and variance bias in Holt-and-Laury lotteries. Applied Economics Letters, 21(1), 35-38.

Holt, C. A. (2007). Markets, games, & strategic behavior. Boston: Pearson Addison Wesley.

Holt, C. A., & Laury, S. K. (2002). Risk aversion and incentive effects. American Economic Review, 92(5), 1644–1655.

Iannello, P., Biassoni F., Nelli, B., Zugno E., & Colombo, B. (2015). The Influence of Menstrual Cycle and Impulsivity on Risk-Taking Behavior. Neuropsychol Trends, 7(17): 47-52.

Jacobson, S., & Petrie, R. (2009). Learning from mistakes: What do inconsistent choices over risk tell us? Journal of risk and uncertainty, 38(2), 143-158.

Johnston, V. S., Hagel, R., Franklin, M., Fink, B., & Grammer, K. (2001). Male facial attractiveness: Evidence for hormone-mediated adaptive design. Evolution and human behavior, 22(4), 251-267.

Kahneman, D. (2011). Pensar rápido, pensar despacio. Barcelona: Debate.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263-291.

Kahneman, D., & Tversky, A. (1984). Choices, values, and frames. American psychologist, 39(4), 341-350.

Kaighobadi, F., & Stevens, J. R. (2013). Does fertility status influence impulsivity and risk taking in human females? Adaptive influences on intertemporal choice and risky decision making. Evolutionary Psychology, 11(3), 700-717.

Kim, Y. E., & Kim, Y. K. (2004). Predicting online purchase intentions for clothing products. European journal of Marketing, 38(7), 883-897.

Klein, G. (2015). Intuición. En Brockman, J. (Ed.). Las mejores decisiones (pp. 43-58). Barcelona, España: Crítica.

Kurath, J., & Mata, R. (2018). Individual differences in risk taking and endogeneous levels of testosterone, estradiol, and cortisol: A systematic literature search and three independent meta-analyses. Neuroscience & Biobehavioral Reviews, 90, 428-446.

Kwon, Y. H., & Shim, S. (1999). A structural model for weight satisfaction, self-consciousness and women’s use of clothing in mood enhancement. Clothing and Textiles Research Journal, 17(4), 203-212.

LaPlante, D. A., Gray, H. M., Bosworth, L., & Shaffer, H. J. (2010). Thirty years of lottery public health research: Methodological strategies and trends. Journal of gambling studies, 26(2), 301-329.

Laury, S. K., & Holt, C. A. (2008). Payoff scale effects and risk preference under real and hypothetical conditions. Handbook of experimental economics results, 1, 1047-1053.

Lazzaro, S. C., Rutledge, R. B., Burghart, D. R., & Glimcher, P. W. (2016). The impact of menstrual cycle phase on economic choice and rationality. PloS one, 11(1), e0144080.

León, O. G. (1987). La toma de decisiones individuales con riesgo desde la psicología. Estudios de psicología, 30, 81-94.

Little, A. C., Jones, B. C., & Burriss, R. P. (2007). Preferences for masculinity in male bodies change across the menstrual cycle. Hormones and Behavior, 51(5), 633-639.

Lucas, M. M., & Koff, E. (2013). How conception risk affects competition and cooperation with attractive women and men. Evolution and Human Behavior, 34(1), 16-22.

Lucas, M. M., Koff, E., & Skeath, S. (2007). Pilot study of relationship between fertility risk and bargaining. Psychological Reports, 101(1), 302-310.

Mani, S. (2008). Progestin receptor subtypes in the brain: the known and the unknown. Endocrinology, 149(6), 2750-2756.

McEwen, B. S. (2002). Estrogen actions throughout the brain. Recent progress in hormone research, 57, 357-384.

McEwen, B. S., & Alves, S. E. (1999). Estrogen actions in the central nervous system. Endocrine reviews, 20(3), 279-307.

Melo, L. (1993). Los incentivos monetarios en la economía experimental: un estudio de caso. Desarrollo y sociedad, 31, 91-120.

Mishra, S. (2014). Decision-making under risk: Integrating perspectives from biology, economics, and psychology. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 18(3), 280-307.

Misiego, F., & de la Morena, A. (2016). ¿Compras con el corazón o el cerebro? Neuromarketing, sensory y neuromanagement para mujeres y hombres curiosos. Bogotá: Rasche.

Mitchell, V. W., & Walsh, G. (2004). Gender differences in German consumer decision-making styles. Journal of Consumer Behaviour, 3(4), 331-346.

Morgenstern, O. (1955). La teoría de los juegos y del comportamiento económico. Disponible en: http://sedici.unlp.edu.ar/handle/10915/8865

Nielsen. (2016). Comportamiento del consumo de la mujer colombiana en belleza y salud. Recuperado de https://goo.gl/1632lK

Pawlowski, B., & Jasienska, G. (2005). Women’s preferences for sexual dimorphism in height depend on menstrual cycle phase and expected duration of relationship. Biological psychology, 70(1), 38-43.

Pearson, M., & Schipper, B. C. (2013). Menstrual cycle and competitive bidding. Games and Economic Behavior, 78(1), 1-20.

Penton-Voak, I. S., & Perrett, D. I. (2000). Female preference for male faces changes cyclically: Further evidence. Evolution and Human Behavior, 21(1), 39-48.

Pine, K. J., & Fletcher, B. C. (2011). Women’s spending behaviour is menstrual-cycle sensitive. Personality and individual differences, 50(1), 74-78.

Pocock, G., & Richards, C. D. (2005). Fisiología humana: la base de la medicina. Madrid: Masson.

Protopopescu, X., Pan, H., Altemus, M., Tuescher, O., Polanecsky, M., McEwen, B., ... & Stern, E. (2005). Orbitofrontal cortex activity related to emotional processing changes across the menstrual cycle. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences of the United States of America, 102(44), 16060-16065.

Puts, D. A. (2005). Mating context and menstrual phase affect women’s preferences for male voice pitch. Evolution and Human Behavior, 26(5), 388-397.

Real Academia Española. (2001). Diccionario de la lengua española. (22ª. ed.). Madrid: Espasa Calpe.

Redolar-Ripoll, D., & Tobeña, A. (2014). Conducta sexual, cerebro, cognición y afectividad. En: Redolar-Ripoll, D. (Ed.), Neurociencia cognitiva. (pp. 575-599). Madrid, España: Médica Panamericana.

Rich, S. U., & Jain, S. C. (1968). Social class and life cycle as predictors of shopping behavior. Journal of Marketing Research, 5(1)41-49.

Rizzo, D. C. (2011). Fundamentos de anatomía y fisiología. (3a. ed.). México: Cengage Learning.

Saad, G., & Stenstrom, E. (2012). Calories, beauty, and ovulation: The effects of the menstrual cycle on food and appearance-related consumption. Journal of Consumer Psychology, 22(1), 102-113.

Seock, Y. K., & Bailey, L. R. (2008). The influence of college students’ shopping orientations and gender differences on online information searches and purchase behaviours. International Journal of Consumer Studies, 32(2), 113-121.

Silverthorn, D. U. (2008). Fisiología humana: un enfoque integrado (4a. Ed.). Argentina: Médica Panamericana.

Simon, H. A. (1955). A Behavioral Model of Rational Choice. The Quarterly Journal of Economics, 69(1), 99-118.

Simon. H. A. (1956). Rational choice and the structure of the environment. Psychological Review; 63(2), 129-138.

Small, C. M., Manatunga, A. K., & Marcus, M. (2007). Validity of self-reported menstrual cycle length. Annals of Epidemiology, 17(3), 163-170.

Stanovich, K. E., & West, R. F. (2000). Individual Differences in Reasoning: Implications for the rationality debate. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 23(5), 645-665.

Stanton, A. A. (2010). Hormonal influence on male decision-making: Implications for organizational management. In: Stanton, A. A., Day, M., & Welpe, I. M. (Eds.). Neuroeconomics and the Firm (pp. 131-150). Massachusetts, USA: Edward Elgar Publishing.

Stanton, S. J. (2017). The role of testosterone and estrogen in consumer behavior and social & economic decision making: A review. Hormones and Behavior, 92, 155-163.

Stanton, S. J., & Edelstein, R. S. (2009). The physiology of women’s power motive: implicit power motivation is positively associated with estradiol levels in women. Journal of Research in Personality, 43(6), 1109–1113.

Stanton, S. J., & Schultheiss, O. C. (2007). Basal and dynamic relationships between implicit power motivation and estradiol in women. Hormones and Behavior, 52(5), 571–580.

Thornhill, R., Gangestad, S. W., Miller, R., Scheyd, G., McCollough, J. K., & Franklin, M. (2003). Major histocompatibility complex genes, symmetry, and body scent attractiveness in men and women. Behavioral Ecology, 14(5), 668-678.

Tversky, A. (1972). Choice by elimination. Journal of Mathematical Psychology, 9(4), 341-367.

Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1974). Judgment under uncertainty: heuristics and biases. Biases in judgments reveal some heuristics of thinking under uncertainty. Science, 185(4157), 1124-1131.

Van den Bos, R., den Heijer, E., Vlaar, S., & Houx, B. B. (2007). Exploring gender differences in decision-making using the Iowa Gambling Task. In Elsworth, J. E. (Ed.). Psychology of Decision Making in Education, Behavior, and High Risk Situations (pp. 207–226). Hauppage, New York: Nova Science Publishers.

Vélez Pareja, I. A. (2002). Decisiones de inversión. Enfocado a la valoración de empresas. (3ª. ed.). Bogotá: Centro Editorial Javeriano.

Viveros Fuentes, S., Nuñez Herrejón, J. L., & Ortiz Salinas, M. E. (2010). APA diccionario conciso de Psicología. México: Manual Moderno.

Vohs, K. D., & Faber, R. J. (2007). Spent resources: Self-regulatory resource availability affects impulse buying. Journal of consumer research, 33(4), 537-547.

Von Stengel, B. (2007). Equilibrium computation for Two-player games in strategic and extensive form. In Nisan, N., Roughgarden, T., Tardos, E., & Vazirani, V. (Eds.), Algorithmic (pp. 53-78). New York, United States: Cambridge University Press.

Wheeler, S. C., & Berger, J. (2007). When the same prime leads to different effects. Journal of Consumer Research, 34(3), 357-368.

Wilcox, A. J., Baird, D. D., Dunson, D. B., McConnaughey, D. R., Kesner, J. S., & Weinberg, C. R. (2004). On the frequency of intercourse around ovulation: evidence for biological influences. Human Reproduction, 19(7), 1539-1543.

Wilcox, A. J., Duncan, D. B., Weinberg, C. R., Trussell, J., & Baird, D. D. (2001). Likelihood of conception with a single act of intercourse: Providing benchmark rates for assessment of post-coital contraceptives. Contraception, 63, 211–215.